top of page

Sonic spatiality: 

a perspective on compositional expression of space 

in electroacoustic music

Abstract

The purpose of this commentary is to examine spatial reasoning in the act of listening to electroacoustic music through surveying various perspectives on the compositional expression of spaces. The presence of space in electroacoustic music is ubiquitous and can be examined through both measurable and objective data as well as through subjective metaphysical descriptions. Due to the performance practices of the electroacoustic medium, the spatial deduction plays a key role in the listener’s appreciation. To utilise this influence in electroacoustic compositional strategies, space should be considered through rigorous analysis and various expressions in compositions.

This commentary will discuss the philosophical and aesthetic signification of space and place. Furthermore, the way in which space can be transformed into its different qualities will be demonstrated in three compositional implementations: spatial integration and disintegration, extension and migration. Thus, the compositions introduce diverse perspectives on space and invite sounds to travel actively or passively around the composed landscape.

Table of Contents

Introduction

- Approach

Methodology

1. Space-place Reasoning and Sound Transformation

1.1. Space and Place

Space-place continuum

1.2. Spatial reasoning / Space and Place in Electroacoustic Music

1.2.1. Spatiality in music

1.2.2. Spatiomorphology

1.2.3. Space-Forming: the primary stage of spatial reasoning

1.2.4. Meaning: Complexity of the reasoning

Schema

Schema in listening

Blending

Summary

1.3. Sound Transformation

Integration and disintegration

Extension and Expansion

Migration

1.4. Virtual space and sound representation

2. Compositions

2.1. Atmosphere (2016)

2.2. Nong-Hyun (2017)

2.3. S Quartet (2017)

2.4. A Trace of Directionality (2018)

2.5. Echoes (2019)

2.6. Teles (2019)

Conclusion

Bibliography

Introduction

New technology has given us the opportunity to enter into a new phase of aural space and enabled creative potential to develop electroacoustic art. Composers of electroacoustic music have departed not only from restrictions of traditional composition methods, instruments, performers and venues, but also from the traditional acoustic space. Using recorded sound as material, for example, electroacoustic music can illuminate musically-familiar features such as pitch or rhythm as well as the referential or external attributes of sound. Moreover, multichannel sound systems in the electroacoustic discipline can encourage both telepresence in the listener and allow near-endless possibilities for artistic expression through the spatial attributes of sound.

Many electroacoustic compositions have dealt with space by their own unique manners in various formats such as acousmatic fixed media, live performances, audio-visual works or installations. While listening to the compositions, one may imagine some images as a spatial condition that may be a re-creation of his/her memory or a compound of it. In other words, the proposed sounds, as physical vibrations traveling through a medium, affect and change the space reflected to the audiences’ mind in various identities.

In Luc Ferrari’s Tautologos III (2005) for piano, viola and electronics, for example, the multidimensional aspects of space appear dramatically. There are two rules to perform the score: each performer can freely choose and play a theme and interval of silence, and then repeat it for twenty-one minutes. They can also maintain or alter their choice dependent on the other performer’s playing. The whole process will be recorded repeatedly, supported by a tape part. The piano and viola play traditional tonal music, while tape parts consist of various sound samples including a bandoneon, voice, book, percussion instruments, etc. Each passage has its own unique landscape with different emotional qualities that I can imagine visually. Specifically, in the forefront of the music, the appearance of a piano part (0’12”) evokes certain romantic or melancholic feelings, which is followed by a violin part (0’23”) with similar emotion. However, we can imagine distinct and specific landscapes from those individual emotional qualities because of different musical style and transposed passages. Furthermore, the dissonance of the musical chord at the end of each passage (0’18” and 0’29”) may encourage a sudden change between piano and violin, which amplifies the contrast of those musical images. The following series of juxtaposed recorded sound clips (as even more disparate sounds) render the dramatic changes of acoustic landscapes. It may be because its musical properties such as rhythm or pitch, its implication of human agency regarding spectral and morphological changes, or its semiotic images of the sound origins derived from my empirical knowledge or memory. I may shift my focus on individual recordings to chase the musical narrative as an emotional journey, or otherwise form a holistic image by layering the sounds montages. Thus, the imagination in the music can be described as blended space, a subset of both preceding schemas with regard to some set of common dimensions. 

This listening process demonstrates that my listening to electroacoustic music is a sonic journey, traveling to various virtual places by virtue of imagination. It also shows that sound penetrates from the outside to the inside space of my mind and vice versa as a continuous line connecting between the two different spaces. As Bridger (1993) asserts, electroacoustic music may be “more directly suggestive of pictorial imagery than other music”.[1] This is because imagination is a form of experience inclusive of our visual sense and also because “the listening to electroacoustic music is as much an act of reasoning as of imagining”, as proposed by Suk-Jun Kim (2010).[2]

It appears that the ways in which we perceive space in electroacoustic music are highly complex. While the concept of ‘space’ as derived from semiotic listening is distinct from space in spectromorphological listening, our actual perceptions of space often waver in the continuum between the two.[3] Exploring this continuum may highlight the distinction of these different types of ‘spaces’, and more importantly, assist listeners to concentrate both on the spectral sounds and their temporal alterations, and also on their perceived relationship between the abstract and the mimetic, body and non-body, space and place.[4][5][6] Therefore, it is important to propose a ‘road map’ for such sonic journeys by conducting research on spatial reasoning through various forms of compositions that concern space in diverse approaches and in unique perspectives. My portfolio of compositions, put together as part of my PhD study, are some examples of such compositions. In this commentary, I aim to explain the diverse approaches and unique perspectives I have taken with my compositions.

 

Approach

It is often considered that space is an absolute and a priori concept in our human experience. As beings with a physical body, we can interpret and measure space by orientation, depth and size in relation to ourselves as a central point. The abstract notion of space may unconsciously permeate our mind because of its absoluteness. In the arts as well as in modern science, measurable geometrical space has been an essential value for human creations. Even in Western instrumental or vocal music, composers may consider the spatial occupancy of their music, for example, singular notes or rich and full harmonies, single performer or large orchestra. Moreover, the acoustics of the performance venue influences the quality of the sound, influencing the audience’s perception of the composed space of the music.

My compositions deal with the transformation of acoustic space as well as the journey to imagined sonic landscapes formed by the listener. This portfolio is based on the premise that all auditory stimuli intrinsically contain spatial information. The precondition demands a specific classification on the use of the terms ‘space’ and ‘place’: space as the concept of geometrical, absolute and universal space which exists separately from our experience, or place as a belonged and connected site – a subjective, holistic view developed from one’s personal experiences with said site. In electroacoustic music, especially acousmatic medium, the landscape is less conspicuous as intended because of a lack of references such as visual, memory and contextual clues. In contrast, anecdotal music or soundscape compositions provide clear indications of their compositional setting, often based on society’s collective knowledge. Both processes are closely linked with schemata that significantly influence our meaning-making as well as spatial reasoning.

This research aims to examine how wide ranges in spatial reasoning emerge during the act of listening to electroacoustic music. It also hopes to demonstrate how sonic transformations can cause the listener’s perceptions of space to shift. Thus, this approach uses diverse compositional strategies with electroacoustic music.

 

Methodology

The acoustic relationship of the space-place continuum is not clearly distinguished in daily life but, in electroacoustic music, sound transformation may shift the listener’s focus and perspective consciously from ordinary to specialised listening. In my compositions, I will highlight the relationship between the proposed various spatial imaginations in three ways. Firstly, I will attempt to disassemble the elements of space through temporal or spectral disintegration and integration of sound with the intentions of shifting the perspective from semantic to spectral factors. Secondly, I will extend and expand conceptual images and roles of an instrument and stage in live performances. Finally, I will use a sound to evoke an image of a place, which will be contrasted against another imagined place inferred by the same sound by using other sounds as contextual clues. The overlap between spatial images will produce a new perspective on the intersectional sound of those places. By these methods, my works show spatial journeys through the transformation of the space-place continuum in order to effectively illuminate unique emotions, as well as the meaning derived through schematic networks of overlapping spaces.

 

[1] Michael Bridger, ‘Narrativisation in Electroacoustic and Computer Music—Reflections on Empirical Research into Listeners’ Response’, International Computer Music Conference (Tokyo, 1993), pp.296-299 (p.296).

[2] Suk-Jun, Kim, ‘Acousmatic Reasoning: An Organised Listening with Imagination’, p.11.

[3] Ibid., p.3.

[4] Simon Emmerson, ‘The Relation of Language to Materials’, in Simon Emmerson (ed.), The Language of Electroacoustic Music (London: The Macmillan Press Ltd, 1986), pp.17–39 (p.29).

[5] Suk-Jun, Kim, ‘Imaginal Listening: a quaternary framework for listening to electroacoustic music and phenomena of sound-images’, Organised Sound, 15/1 (2010), pp.43-53, (p.45).

[6] Yi Fu Tuan, Space and Place: The Perspective of Experience, (the University of Minnesota Press, 2001/8th edn), p.6.

1. Space-place reasoning and Sound Transformation

1.1. Space and Place

The use of space is mixed with a place in everyday experience. Although space and place are not a separate notion, the concepts of space and place that the philosophers refer to have distinct features. Even to this day, we may be influenced by the Newtonian scientific materialism, absolute space which slaughters individuality and richness of human experience although we never witness it, instead we only encounter space as a form of place in our lives. However, the concept of place began to gain interest again amongst 20th century philosophers Whitehead, Husserl and Merleau-Ponty. According to them, the role of a body is essential to reveal the place, meaning that, the perceiving subject is ultimately the perceived world.[1] Therefore, my body as a personal possession is now the centre of our perceptual phenomenon in modern philosophy and place has reclaimed its value as the ultimate locus where we are residing and engaging with other bodies in the world. Thus, Casey argues;

The places we inhabit are known by the bodies we live. Moreover, we cannot be implaced without being embodied. Conversely, to be embodied is to be capable of implacement.[2]

The purpose of focusing on a body is that space exists outside of our bodily existence, while place is a metaphysical site as an object we create. More specifically, all sensual existence such as colour, sound and tactile stimuli (as non-context embedded entities) could be a fundamental element of spatiality and we may also perceive those geometric patterns of nature through our senses and create abstract space with undefined feelings such as pleasant/unpleasant, surprise or fear in our mind. Then, a concrete emotional reality (place as a context-embedded entity) is acquired through the desire to understand the relationship between the fragments of the sense data. In this regard, however, one thing we should clarify is that the abstractness and concreteness will vary depending on our unique perceptions. Space is abstract when viewed from our subjective perspective, but it appears concretely when it is objectified and shared by individuals. For example, we can share the notion of spatial sound qualities by the elaborated taxonomy of spectromorphologies, but the quality itself can be abstract to the subject because of the many possibilities for it to be embodied in various ways.

Conversely, places evoke certain emotions which could appear very concrete to us, while our own experiences are different and abstract from those of others – the place of my home is very specific to me both structurally and emotionally, but not to others. Bachelard assigned “the intimate values of inside space” as a face of poetic images into place. This intimate space is separate from geometric space, which is irrelevant to the subject’s individuality. This is because our own worlds depend on the image created by subject themselves as much as it relies on the fact, that is, “when the image is new, the world is new”.[3]

As we can see with Bachelard’s approach, our empirical knowledge is a key factor for conceptualisation. Space is an objective and absolute concept which may be less influenced by our individuality, whereas place is a subjective and empirical site influenced by our own accumulated knowledge. However, we tend to have unconditional belief in space because of its absoluteness as opposed to an arbitrary place. As a matter of fact, we do not see space in reality, rather it may be inferred from the experience of a place which we encounter every day. Tuan defined the place as a type of non-physical object and exemplified the idea of space and place with the recognition of an object;

Places and objects define space, giving it a geometric personality. Neither the newborn infant nor the man who gains sight after a lifetime of blindness can immediately recognize a geometric shape such as a triangle. The triangle is at first "space," a blurred image. Recognizing the triangle requires the prior identification of corners—that is, places.[4]

The genuine triangle only exists in our Platonic ideas. We are not only familiar to the concept of a triangle but can also easily find and distinguish its variations in reality by eidetic intuition[5]. In other words, a concept (space) is abstracted from our experience of it (place) and “when space feels thoroughly familiar to us, it has become place”.[6]

 

Space-place continuum

The Space-place continuum has been introduced by geographical studies for new geographic information technologies.[7] Edwardes and Purves (2007) describe place as experiential space located at “the opposite end of a continuum of geographic perspectives from space” (see Figure 1).[8] Specifically for them, place is;

most distinct at the start, relating geography to human existence, experiences and interaction. At the other end is the more detached, abstract and objective view of space, ultimately represented as geometry, which provides a means to think about, describe and encode the world in a logical way.[9]

 

Figure 1. The space-place continuum (from Edwardes, 2007)


 The citation seems to be analogous with the distinction of space and place, which gives support for the philosophical approach’s effect on multidisciplinary studies. In their research, Edwardes and Purves believe the approach will suggest new perspectives on places from ontological to epistemological categories. Likewise, this notion indicates to electroacoustic music composers and listeners that acousmatic reasoning could lie on a perceptual continuum between informative and summative places. Thus, the sound transformation can change and assist the listener’s perspectives in traversing the grey area as well as the extremes of the space and place.

 

 

[1] Maurice Merleau-Ponty, Phenomenology of Perception, (Routledge, 2002/2nd edn), p. 83.

[2] Edward S. Casey, The Fate of Place: A Philosophical History, (Berkeley, Los Angeles, California: University of California Press, 1997), p.233.

[3] Gaston Bachelard, The Poetics of Space, (New York: Penguin Books, 2014/2nd edn), p.68.

[4] Tuan, Space and Place: The Perspective of Experience, p.17.

[5] “Husserl’s term, Eidetic intuition, denotes the constitution of a certain kind of knowledge, which is rather different from the awareness of this and that, from the acquaintance with things of everyday life, and from empirical knowledge. Eidetic intuition, or catching sight of what things are themselves essentially, lies at the very heart of phenomenology and is therefore knit together with phenomenological reduction and other central features of this philosophy.”, Friedrich A. Uehlein, ‘Eidos and Eidetic Variation in Husserl’s Phenomenology’, in Manfred Spitzer, Friedrich A. Uehlein, et al. (ed.), Phenomenology, Language & Schizophrenia (Springer, New York, 1992), p.88-102 (p.88).

[6] Tuan, Space and Place: The Perspective of Experience, p.73.

[7] Alistair Edwardes and Ross S. Purves, ‘A Theoretical Grounding for Semantic Descriptions’, in J. Mark Ware, George E. Taylor (ed.), Web and Wireless Geographical Information Systems, 7th International Symposium, (Springer, Cardiff, 2007), pp.106-120 (pp.118-119).

[8] Alistair Edwardes and Ross S. Purves, ‘Exploiting Volunteered Geographic Information to describe Place’, GIS Research UK 16th Annual Conference (2008), p.1.

[9] Edwardes and Purves, ‘A Theoretical Grounding for Semantic Descriptions’, 2007, p.109.

spacePlaceContinuum.jpg

1.2. Spatial reasoning / Space and place in electroacoustic music

 

Attempts to separate our auditory sense from other senses through acousmatic music have led to in-depth concerns about our acoustic reasoning. The ability to perceive sounds in various ways without external stimuli was, initially, the most basic and interesting motive in this study. For example, we experience a perceptual transition from an accelerated rhythm to a pitched sound and a chord created by gathering several notes into a single sonic event (a sound-image), meaning that we assume a qualitative difference from a quantitative difference. By pursuing this, sound - like a bowl - can contain our sensory associations, feelings and emotions of our present experience, which builds strong bonds in our memory as schemata. For example, language as a complex sound form can be said to be a container of our senses, knowledge and our life with which we communicate in society. Therefore, in the acoustic world, existence does not come from the vibrational propagation from the sound’s origin, but instead is a conceptual embodiment influenced by our individual senses and impressions. As humans, we can summon such events to our mind through sound. 

In this study, space is described and divided into two concepts: space as the empty space of the sound bowl (an experience dependent on our senses, a one-dimensional description) and place, the element that fills up space with various experiences (an experience of our imagination[1]). All of my compositions have perceptual conflicts of spatial deduction between not only space and place, but also place and place. The former confrontation may be easily found from the relation between aural and mimetic discourses in electroacoustic music. On the other hand, deduction from sound alone could arouse different consequence for defining a place. In Teles (2019), for example, there is a ‘water-falling’ sound, and it may result in different images for the listeners such as raining or a waterfall. To make sure of the spatial judgement, they need to search for context from other sounds such as a roll of thunder in advance (for raining), or dried footstep and skateboard sounds juxtaposed with the ‘water-falling’ sound (for waterfall). This process is required to concentrate on the spatiality of sound itself, that is, space. The suggested contextual clues may influence the spatial reasoning for listeners, which may result in them reconsidering the ‘water-falling’ sound.

The two experiences of space and place seem to be markedly different in their definitions. Nevertheless, to demonstrate their essential commonality, the sonic experiences of space and place should be described as a continuum rather than as completely different entities. Intentionality proposes that, in our reasoning, “all consciousness is about or of something”.[2] The directivity implies the act of pointing somewhere that our consciousness is moving, and also implies that there is a departure and a destination. This suggests that there are different states of existence whether they are spiritual or material. In other words, every sensation is spatial.[3] However, space and place may still be distinguishable by whether the sensation targets a physical existence or mental references formed by a schema in our memory. Referring to our memory, we may have rich impressions of the sound as if recalling a place in the past. Whereas, focusing on our senses, we may find an emptiness in the sound, that is, a certain desire to query the sound qualities in order to form a context through which we can understand the circumstances. ‘Sound is a bowl’, as described in the metaphor, the space - the emptiness of bowl as abstractness - would be the essence of aesthetic expression in acousmatic music.

 

1.2.1. Spatiality in music

Music as a temporal art form (Zeitkunst) consists of intervals between notes or events. In harmony, or even two-tone combinations as a method of measuring the phenomenon of consonance-dissonance, we might say there is space between not only a note and the successive (melody), but also a note and the simultaneous (harmony). The meaning of ‘interval’ is not merely interpreted as a span, gap or interstice between or within determinate entities, but also implies spatial distance as a prerequisite of co-existence. In addition, the co-existence can also involve the relation between the sound and the perceiver. In many languages, the sense of distance creates a number of words about the spatial divisions such as personal, demonstrative pronouns and local adverbs - ‘us’ and ‘them’, ‘this’ and ‘that’, or ‘closely’ and ‘distantly’, which tend to signify relative distances from oneself. The meanings of these are closely related and mutually intertwined with the complexity of interpersonal intimacy and geographical distance.[4] These words essentially imply natural scientific principles in an acoustic world, such as that ‘the nearer the sounding origin, the louder the sound’ – in other words, the musical dynamics connote the sense of distance. Piaget and Inhelder (1967) insist that this principle of spatial relationship is essential for human beings and related study has been examined by many scholars.[5] As the music moved into a new epoch of electroacoustic medium, the notion of the spatial quality of sound has been broadened because sound is not only able to capture places by recording technology, but can also produce virtual spatiality. 

 

1.2.2. Spatiomorphology

Smalley focused on the intrinsic spatiality of sound by spectromorphology to contribute to building a framework for understanding the structural relations and behaviours of sound. According to Smalley, spatial conditions and motions determine spectromorphological factors which are in an inseparable relation with our spatial perception. His term spatiomorphology emphasises the unique focus on spatial attributes easily overlooked by listeners as a sound in itself.[6],[7] While mentioning the term spatiomorphology, he provided detailed categorisations into two generic concepts of spaces; composed spaces as “composed onto the recording media” and listening spaces in which “the composed space is heard”[8] This catergorisation is followed by well-organised subordinate concepts; internal and external spaces, and diffused and personal spaces derived from composed and listening spaces respectively (1991). In his writing, Smalley gave a lot of weight to the significance of external spaces “which is made apparent to us through reflections” and is described as a perspectival space – similar to visual space, where a “listener can apprehend spaces much broader than the real” by looking through loudspeakers as windows.[9] This taxonomy seems to give an elaborate objective portrait of the acoustic world and is a great analytical tool in the methodology to uncover the building blocks of the spatiality in electroacoustic music. Moreover, he extended the spatial perspective to include ‘a more integrated ‘ecological’ approach’ because he believes that ‘we cannot separate space itself from what produces it, nor from our experience of space in nature and culture’.[10] His Orbieu soundscape shows the detailed spatial description of the place in the spectromorphological approach, that is, the soundscape as an empirical ‘place’ interprets spatial qualities as a reasoned ‘space’ by a rigorous examination. In his reasoning, Smalley differentiated the space-form processes into three types; the ‘journey’ between spaces, an ‘holistic view’ and complex appearance of spaces (mixed forms of ‘journey’ and ‘holistic view’).[11] The journey could be a traditional narrative approach, which completes the storyline by the temporal process of switching the listener’s perspective.  Conversely, the holistic view connotes a certain acoustic place where each audible element and event is temporally accumulated and layered to form a still-frame in the listener’s mind. These perspectival processes are dependent subject’s intentionality.

 

1.2.3. Space-Forming: the primary stage of spatial reasoning

The microscopic and macroscopic perspectives on Orbieu soundscape are also applicable toward the majority of our music appreciation. Let us look into a simple example of the perceptual grouping in musical discourse. In Messiaen’s Mode de valeurs et d’intensités (1949) from Quatre études de rhythm, the jumping notes, from the very beginning, show the spectral intervals between different pitches, which may evoke a sense of elevation. Also, the continuous alteration of each note’s dynamic may imply a sense of its spatial proximity and size. We may listen to the spectromorphological changes by concentrating on the quality of each note.  On the other hands, the spatial qualities of the notes, mostly resonating in the upper register, could be connected and fused together in a small group by virtue of a low tone that conveys the richness of its harmonic overtone and long duration compared to the others. This incorporation may create a landscape in which the individual pitches, as though crystallised sounds, form into a sensuous and emotional place in our mind. John Young (2016) suggests that the intervallic grouping could themselves be considered ‘moments’, which are one of four perspectives on form: musical rhetoric, moment, morphic and narrative.[12] While suggesting these ideas, he stresses the necessity to redefine the idea of form as “a more organic principle by which sound identities” for electroacoustic music rather than the traditional form as “a set of formulaic moulds that simply ‘contain’ music”.[13] The unstable and fragile flexibility of creating form is a natural phenomenon from human beings’ attempt to understand what we perceive, especially in music as a digital representation that is “the most abstract and context free form it has ever taken”.[14]

In phenomenological description, ‘to be perceived’ is ‘to be distinguished’. We cannot have any perception in homogeneous area. Merleau-Ponty described the phenomena as “The perceptual ‘something’ is always in the middle of something else, it always forms art of a ‘field’”. By having a rigorous reasoning of phenomena, when a ‘shape’ has its contour, it can be detached from its background, that is, our forming of the shape creates a space with a shape and background – space must exist by co-existence[15].

From Messiaen’s music, one may not see or deduce space as a general form. Similarly, the act of listening to electroacoustic music, especially for those who are unfamiliar with the genre, remains disorientating and difficult because of its abstract conception or myriad possibilities to build context and a sonic landscape. The concept of ‘form’, suggested by Young, attempts to identify and analyse particular features of sounds and those relationships in electroacoustic music which “frequently resists traditional reductive methods”.[16] I believe that the recognition of form is the initial step of spatial reasoning from sound.

 

1.2.4. Meaning: complexity of the reasoning

Truax claimed that the traditional abstract-oriented aesthetic in Western art practice concerns only internal complexity, which is the abstract structure of its materials and forms. He highlights the importance of taking account of ‘external complexity’ to maintain the balance and synergy with ‘inner complexity’, which will renew the appreciation of listener’s aesthetical response in electroacoustic music.[17] His proposed works of both ‘electroacoustic music theatre’ and ‘soundscape composition’ are context-embedded approaches to music creation, which are more dependent on the listener’s individual perspective on the connections between the sound materials. These perspectives are enclosed in his/her empirical evidence, knowledge. By using recognisable sounds, the forming process may become apparent in our mind. However, the formed soundscape appears to be a highly complicated process owing to the semiotic listening, which combines source-identification with the context-identification processes.[18] The moments, as semiotic entities, could collide or juxtapose with each other, which will produce different contextual meaning depending on the sound disposition in the individual’s listening experience.

The crux of the complexity may originate in the fact that an auditory stimulus cannot be completely detached from other sensations of a whole experience. Transmodal perception has been used to explain the complexity of spatial attributes. It had been regarded as the realm of subjectivity, but the neuroscientific evidence has probed the association that the processing models, such as learning, are the process of connections between neurons which control entire human sensation.[19] Every sensation contains intrinsic signification as an intentional object constituted by our body, pure motor intentionality.[20] The intentional object is not exposed clearly to our consciousness, that is, the meaning of the object can be varied individually because of the contextual dependency of perception that every sensation happens with another sensation, even though the act of listening to acousmatic music explores the primacy of the ear. The subject has formed a certain schema of any sensation and produced meaning out of schematic networks.

 

Schema

The term is frequently referred to in various disciplines, such as psychology, pedagogy and computer science, as a mental structure representing some aspect of the world. Each interpretation among the studies showed inconsistency, which was inevitable, considering the diverse usage of term in its history. Although its ambiguity has often been problematic for researchers, there is little debate over the function of schemas. Generally, the functions that can be discerned are the following: ‘facilitating encoding of new information, including inferential elaboration’, ‘guiding behaviour’ and ‘expediting retrieval processes (i.e., memory search and reconstruction)’.[21]

In terms of the function related to the encoding of new information, Piaget introduced schemas to the field of developmental psychology. In particular, he was interested in the education of children and believed the significance of the intellectual growth of children to be different from that of adults, who have strong existing schematic networks, i.e. an empirical knowledge of the world.[22] According to him, children employ the processes of assimilation and accommodation, adapting a new sensation to an existing schema and modifying the existing schema to embrace the new unclassifiable sensation, forming a schema for the perception and interpretation of their experience. Since Piaget’s theory, the concept has been described with various terminologies, including 'frame' and 'scene and script'. Minsky, a pioneer of artificial intelligence, attempted to develop a computational human-like mechanism through his ‘frame' construct, an extension and elaboration of the schema construct. Rumelhart extended Minsky’s idea and developed the notion of schema explicitly. According to the schema theory of Rumelhart, when people gain knowledge or information, they try to connect it with some structure in their memory so as to better understand it. In addition, individuals split knowledge or information into understandable pieces, categorising them to store and recall them later. Therefore, when individuals learn new knowledge, it can be processed in either a pre-existing schema or a newly formed one. Schemata are therefore mental structures that help humans to grasp new information and effectively recollect and use pre-existing ideas. However, our reliance on schemas could lead us to miss the details of the sensations from our experience. If the new information is similar to the pre-existing data, we can be easily prone to impute the new sensations to a component of the solid schema – that is why, in listening to electroacoustic music, we tend to rely more upon symbolic meaning than our senses as they tend to neglect the spatial attributes of space from listening to sound.

 

Schemata in listening

Figure 2. Five layers in relation to memory system. (Kendall 2014)

Kendall often referred to schemata to account for the listening processes of electroacoustic music. He used the word as an umbrella term and employed it to look closely at the mental phenomena in his five layers that exist in listening processes; Sensation, Gist, Locus, Contexts, and Domains (Figure 2). The schema plays a particularly active role across these layers, and the ways in which the sensory stimulation triggers schemas and schematic connections appear to be fundamental to the meaning-making process.[23] For example, to illustrate the schema in electroacoustic music, these five mental layers might be applied in the analysis of hypothetical hearing of Jonty Harrison’s Klang (1982). At the beginning of the piece, a new perceptual event appears as a pitched, percussive and sustained sound from the perspective of Sensations. The event prompts the listener to search for an overlap between its mimesis and schemas in order to comprehend it, which calculates an assumption of the event as a kind of bell or instrumental resonance by an anonymous performer in a very silent place at Gist. After a few seconds, Locus shifts the subject’s focus from the previously succeeding percussive sounds to a new gestural sound—a covering lid—and has a small certainty that the gestures of bell-like sound act differently from an actual ‘bell’, which does not have a lid. Now the schema of an earthenware casserole dish appears derived from long-term memory by virtue of reducing the ambiguity of bell-like sounds at Locus. Generally, the earthenware is not an instrument and is used in the kitchen for cooking, that is, it has specific schematic spatial attributes and the listener allows the contradiction of the usage in electroacoustic medium at Contexts. Thus, internal representation caused by sensory stimulation eliminates irrelevant schemata from each layer, which prompts the listener to focus on the artistic expression and its feeling in order to bring a sense of consistency to the electroacoustic listening - Kendall calls it ‘harmonisation’. Since the beginning, the structure of the piece has gradually increased from simple and intermittent pitched sounds to a variety of textural and timbral sounds through the interventions of other instruments and digitally processed sound. Contexts anticipate what is to come during the structural developments based on the schematic patterns learned earlier in its process. The comparison between this piece and the schematic knowledge of other electroacoustic music, which displays similar processes may be implemented by Domains. Therefore, each layer of the listening processes activates schemata, which may in turn activate other schemata throughout the layers. While listening, the sounds access the schemata from long-term memory, and are stored in working memory while they are active and organising within mental spaces. Therefore, we expect that any perceivable auditory stimulus will trigger the construction of a mental space in working memory. Then we can say the mental space is as the sound-image landscape: “the imagined source of the perceived sounds defined”.[24] Mental space has features to be linked with other schemata and relies on both the listener’s tendencies and the context—in particular, the context of working memory's other contents, which Kendall called schematic associations.[25]

                                                             

Blending

Fauconnier and Turner (2002) argued a similar idea of meaning formation by the notion of conceptual blending. According to their theory, blending “is a basic mental operation that leads to new meaning”[26], which is the foundation of the meaning-making in daily experience, including the arts.

The essence of the blend is the spontaneous discovery of a third interpretive schema (space/system/mediating coordinate space) that is a subset of both antecedently represented schemas with respect to some set of common dimensions.[27]

They analysed language using a combination of two words as a culturally-formed manner of inventing and disseminating blending schemes. As shown in the four-space model of blending theory, the blend, or new meaning, that is derived from two different input mental spaces (schemata) can be categorised from a generic concept, the generic space (Figure 3). Even though the notion of input can be easily regarded as a simple form or meaning, it is founded on enormous implications, extremely complicated meaning formed or trained by another conceptual blending.

This theory seems to parallel compositional expression in electroacoustic music closely. Kendall’s term harmonisation (2016) may be used not only when the two input mental spaces show a strong schematic association, but also during the incompatible or oppositional blending, which is a weaker network of interconnections where a contradiction will occur. The superimposition of contradictory inputs, however, is an exclusive privilege for composers by electroacoustic means.[28] The conflicting images may evoke more creative and imaginative thinking to construct meaning because the incongruences are highlighted in the process.[29] It is more active, energetic, vibrant, live or attentive than consonance, which has been often employed in the context of contemporary arts[30]: surrealist paintings (e.g. Max Ernst’s works) show the juxta- or superposition of unrelated images. In particular, acoustic information has similar spectromorphological characteristics, which may generate proximity and strong aural interrelationship from such irrelevant semantic combination.[31]McNabb’s Dreamsong (1978), Wishart’s Vox 5 (1986), Normandeau’s Memoires vives (1989) have well indicated the superimposition of different spaces and transformations between them. Kendall applied their approach for the analysis of electroacoustic music and concluded that ‘meaning in electroacoustic music sometimes emerges directly from schematic associations’, and, therefore, the context of an electroacoustic piece can become apparent under the interconnections of two schematic networks.[32] These networks are identical with Fauconnier and Turner’s blended mental space.

Figure 3. Fauconnier and Turner’s four-space model (2002)

 

 

Summary

In this chapter, we briefly examined the structure of knowledge and meaning making within our consciousness. Schema could be described as a pattern of our thought, a bundle of our sense data, of neurons, all compressed in one’s daily experiences. It involves related feelings or emotions, and the encounter or conflict between the schema provides new meaning as well as more established feelings or emotion. From my perspective, I find the parallels between the idea of space-place and schema (which are both perceptual continuums between informative and summative) to be the quantitative difference of our experience – our individuality. This approach allows me to interpret all auditory experiences within the correlation of space and place because schema is concerned with all of our accumulated knowledge and sensory activities.

Each composition has representative schemas in various ways: ‘language’ for Atmosphere, ‘instruments’ for Nong-Hyun, S Quartet and Echoes, ‘stage’ for Connected Islands, ‘a place’ for A trace of directionality and Teles. The schemas have potential to be connected to other schemas in our memory, which is dependent on contextual clues offered by other sounds used in the compositions. Thereby, my composition strategy is to reconstruct and recreate the schematic associations with sound and its transformations. This approach may evoke diverse spatial experiences within the continuum of space or place from the act of listening, which gives new perspective and reconsideration on the sound.

 

[1] I would delimit the imagination which is a spatial and pictorial sense referred to our knowledge and memory.

[2] Shaun Gallagher and Dan Zahavi, The Phenomenological Mind, 2nd edition (Routledge, 2012), p.7.

[3] Merleau-Ponty, Phenomenology of Perception, pp.256–7.

[4] Tuan, Space and Place: The Perspective of Experience, p.47-50.

[5] Curtis Roads, Composing Electronic Music: A New Aesthetic (Oxford University Press, 2015), p.240.

[6] Denis Smalley, ‘Spectromorphology: Explaining Sound-Shapes’, Organised Sound, 2/2 (1997), pp.107–26 (p. 122).

[7] Barry Blesser and Linda-Ruth Salter, Spaces Speak, Are You Listening? Experiencing Aural Architecture (Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press, 2007), p.361.

[8] Smalley, ‘Spectromorphology: Explaining Sound-Shapes’, p.122.

[9] Ibid., p.122.

[10] Ibid., p.54.

[11] Denis Smalley, ‘Space-form and the acousmatic image’, Organised Sound, 12/1 (2007), p.69.

[12] John Young, ‘Forming form’, in Simon Emmerson and Leigh Landy (ed.), Expanding the Horizon of Electroacoustic Music Analysis, (Cambridge University Press, 2016), pp.58-79 (p.68).

[13] Ibid., p.61.

[14] Barry Truax, ‘The Aesthetics of Computer music: A questionable concept reconsidered’, Organised Sound 5/3, 2000, p.120.

[15] Merleau-Ponty, Phenomenology of Perception, pp.256–7.

[16] Young, ‘Forming form’, p.77.

[17] Truax, ‘The Aesthetics of Computer music: A questionable concept reconsidered’, p.126.

[18] Kim, ‘Acousmatic Reasoning: An Organised Listening with Imagination’, p.2.

[19] Timothy A. Keller and Marcel Adam Just, ‘Structural and Functional Neuroplasticity in Human Learning of Spatial Routes’, NeuroImage, 125 (2016), pp.256–66 (p.257).

[20] Merleau-Ponty, Phenomenology of Perception, p. 176.

[21] Vanessa E. Ghosh and Asaf Gilboa, ‘What Is a Memory Schema? A Historical Perspective on Current Neuroscience Literature’, Neuropsychologia, 53 (2014), pp.104–14 (p.105).

[22] 'Jean Piajet', Wikipedia [Website], https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Jean_Piaget&oldid=917602423 (Authored 2019, Accessed 24thOctober 2019).

[23] Gary S. Kendall, ‘Listening and Meaning: How a Model of Mental Layers Informs Electroacoustic Music Analysis’, in Simon Emmerson and Leigh Landy (ed.), Expanding the Horizon of Electroacoustic Music Analysis, (Cambridge University Press, 2016), pp.31-57 (p. 41).

[24] Trevor Wishart, ‘Sound Symbols and Landscapes’, in Simon Emmerson (ed.), The Language of Electroacoustic Music (London: The Macmillan Press Ltd, 1986), pp.41–60 (p.44).

[25] Kendall, ‘Listening and Meaning: How a Model of Mental Layers Informs Electroacoustic Music Analysis’, p.42.

[26] Gilles Fauconnier and Mark Turner, The Way We Think: Conceptual Blending and the Mind’s Hidden Complexities (Basic Books, 2002), p.57.

[27] Mark Turner, The Artful Mind: Cognitive Science and the Riddle of Human Creativity (Oxford University Press, 2006), p.41.

[28] Trevor Wishart and Simon Emmerson, On Sonic Art (Amsterdam: Harwood Academic Publishers, 1996/1st rev.), p.137.

[29] Kendall, ‘Listening and Meaning: How a Model of Mental Layers Informs Electroacoustic Music Analysis’, p.43.

[30] Ibid., p.45.

[31] Wishart, ‘Sound Symbols and Landscapes’, p.43.

[32] Kendall, ‘Listening and Meaning: How a Model of Mental Layers Informs Electroacoustic Music Analysis’, p.43.

schemaEL.png
blended.png

1.3. Sound Transformation

In electroacoustic music, sound transformation is a core methodology and a basic means of compositional articulation.Concerning the transformation, “changes in the state of a sonic identity”[1] shift the listener’s perspective during the transformation converted by the diverse aesthetic intention of a composer toward temporal flux of the music. Sonic flexibility is a unique feature of the electroacoustic medium. Transformation, in a broad sense, is ubiquitous in all sonic arts but the meaning and function may alter according to the musical context during the transformation. Smalley (1993) suggests a perceptual distinction of transformations divided into two types: spectromorphological and source-cause transformations.[2] The spectromorphological transformation can be discovered by concentrated listening to a present sense of sound properties such as pitch, duration, loudness, spectrum, spatial position, etc, whereas the source-cause transformation requires our reasoning to interpret or understand the identity of sounds without conscious effort. From an acousmatic viewpoint, the latter category may fall into the former because spectromorphological changes occur in all sound transformation, that is, the spectromorphologcial transformation intervenes during the source-cause transformation depending on the listener’s perceptual point of view. Sonic identity will change at a perceptual threshold in which sound transformation occurs, although it depends on the degree of intensity. Roads (2015) classified the concept into mild and radical transformation for the disparity. The mild transformation tends to be ornamental and without major perceptual deformation, whereas the radical transformation may cause a drastic structural change for a listener with the obliteration of its perceptual identity.[3] As listening can result in various meanings from a sonic event, the sound continuum is the fundamental concept of sound transformation.

The major influencer of the continuum concept is the liberation from the conceptual oppression of musical components in traditional instrumental or vocal music. Its notation has been developed to contain many possibilities of sound before the electroacoustic music era, but it remains a limited way of expressing meaning. Pitch, for example, is filled by dividing a scale into twelve equally compressed steps, and the length of musical notes combined with the dynamic marks suppress their unlimited capability in a few stages. Moreover, the qualities of sound are defined by certain musical parameters such as rhythm, pitch or timbre. The continuous series of sound varies with a temporal difference from our perceptual awareness.

In 1930, Cowell first described the notion of a rhythmic continuum between infrasonic frequencies (periodic/aperiodic rhythms) and the audible frequencies—pitched/unpitched tones.[4] Each musical element has its own schemata, such as a note, tone, and rhythm, all of which are the results of time-structure. In his serial piece, Kontakte (1960), Stockhausen points out that human perception splits the acoustical frequency continuum into a number of different phenomena.[5]Also, in his focus on the boundary of the note-noise continuum, Smalley referred to the critical point of the ear as the effluvium, and the qualitative change has forced a listener to bring his/her focal strategy away from one another.[6]Moreover, the focal change occurs between different source-causes as sonic entities such as instrumental and vocal, or electronic and recorded sounds by sound transformation. McNabb’s Dreamsong (1978) shows the morphing or interpolation processes between the human voice and mechanical synthesized sound, which allows the listener’s perspective to shift again from mimetic sounds to musical timbre. He explained his intention regarding the work as;

The basic intent of the piece was to integrate a set of synthesized sounds with a set of digitally recorded natural sounds to such a degree that they would form a continuum of available sound material. The sounds thus range from the easily recognisable to the totally new, or, more poetically, from the real world to the dream realm of the imagination, with all that that implies with regard to transitions, recurring elements, and the unexpected.[7]

Conversely, the transformation between voice and twitter, or book and door slams as different objective existences can be found from Wishart’s Red Bird (1977). In his books On sonic art (1985) and Audible Design (1994), Wishart argued the concept of transformation for dealing with sound as a continuum and applied “his theory to techniques of production and manipulation”.[8]

The transformational process involves morphing or interpolation of the sounds, which involves three aspects of sound images: initial identity, its mutation and transmutation. Those progressions and relationships may help audiences find the context of the work, which would lead to meaning making and healthy appreciation in electroacoustic music. In my following portfolio, I attempted to implement three certain types of sound transformations as the methodology for the listener’s perspectival change on the continuum between space and place, and vice versa. 

 

Integration and disintegration

Previously in this chapter, we examined spatial forming as the beginning of spatial reasoning. Integration and disintegration signify the forward and reverse processes of space-forming respectively. The fundamental reunion of the spatial reasoning should reconstitute the sonic identity and our acoustic space. In terms of integration, basic sound components such as spectral elements or morphological gestures becomes a sonic identity or soundscape with a certain schema, that is, certain spectromorphological aspects become a symbolic sound. The process may seem to be a structure of additive synthesis spreading over temporal flow. Conversely, disintegration, as the opposite concept, contains a process in which initial schematic association is disassembled into individual schemata by sound qualities normally masked by its generic concept, and which therefore emerge into the listener’s concentration.

In Alvin Lucier’s I Am Sitting in a Room (1970), the body/place axis, existing as the voice in the room, is gradually disintegrated with its identity. The decoupling process renders the negation of body and place from the distorted semiotic features or sound properties.[9] The perceptual drastic changes of the sound-image enrich our act of imagining, which arouses meaningful appreciation for listening to electroacoustic music. This methodology was taken into account of in the compositional process of Atmosphere (2016) and S-quartet (2017).

 

Extension and Expansion

The other type of transformation is an attempt to expand sound into space or place through functional and physical enlargement of an instrument or listening space. Live performance with an instrument and electronic sound is common compositional discourse in today’s electroacoustic music. Of course, the live performance produces a different aesthetic point of view, which in turn affects our spatial reasoning. The visual information in live instrument performance could show the decoding process of its notation, promoting interaction between the live instrumental and electroacoustic sound world, performance and the audiences. The electronic part could be used not only as decoration for an instrumental line but also as a counterpart in an equivalent position, acting like a duet. Moreover, this collaboration between instrumental performance and electronic music attempts to expand the audience’s perception of space. The listener may primarily concentrate on the sound coming from the stage space from a causal perspective regarding the performer’s movements and expression by means of the visual information. Whereas placing their focus on the overall sonic landscape allows the listener to feel its emotional impression. Emmerson suggested different spatial properties named local and field to describe those functions in live electronic music. In Zellen-Linien (2007) by Hans Tutschku, the listener may recognise perceptual differences between the piano and synthesised (or pre-recorded) sounds and their empirical knowledge. Moreover, the visual clue may help them make this distinction. Regardless of whether the electronic sounds may be similar to the sound of the instrument, we can assume that the sound is derived from the piano by the spectromorphological and contextual relation within the work. Thereby, both the live instrumental and computer-produced sounds are fused along the lines of the performance, which may create the soundscape as a whole. Emmerson mentioned that “the two functions very clearly articulated” the continuum between them.[10] This methodology was taken into account of in the compositional process of the live electroacoustic work, Echoes (2019).

Another goal is to expand the performance space. As we discussed, the main purpose of this study is to change our spatial perspective to the continuously changing landscape of sound. Indeed, the simplest ways to change our current spatiality is by moving our body. In this way, the sound spatiality will change along with the listener’s body movement as their spatial constitutor, even when turning one’s head. In this regard, the point of the expansion is that the work invites audience members to participate in the composition as performers through their movement. Thereby, the spatiomorphological factor will change their appearances and its context of perception simultaneously. That is, the intervention of their body can impact upon the contents of composition for individual listeners. This methodology was taken into account in the compositional process of the installation work, Connected Islands (2019).

 

Migration

Finally, the sound transformation among space and place is implemented by the migration of sound-objects and sound images. Multi-channel representation technology has expanded perspectival space, which facilitates various spatial sound expressions such as the movement between not only left and right, but also front and rear, or up and down through sound distribution that comes from all directions. Since humans walk upright, the flow of their perceptual auditory contents moves from front to back in reality (unless they do not turn their head). However, through sound alone, humans cannot distinguish the front and back as well as they can the sides because of the position of our ears. Rather, there is a little spectral difference by our pinna. The first attempt of this sound migration concerns mainly the front-back movement of sound thorough the spatial multi-channel representation. This methodology was taken into account in the compositional process of the Ambisonic piece, A trace of directionality (2018).

In addition, the symbolic images of sounds are the key clues in our spatial reasoning along with the spectromorphological aspects. Moreover, these images are gathered to reveal a certain place, that is, the image as a common denominator connecting from one place to the other. This approach has the potential to represent diverse places. The compositional strategy through migration is to try various combinations of these images through sound transformation to complete the journey between places. Jonty Harrison’s Sorties (1995), for example, shows the migrational transformation of sonic locations based on the idea of acousmatic travelling. The work provides clear sonic landscapes out of the presented sound images such as heels clicks, subway brakes, door slams, keys rattle, raining, thunder booms, buzzing fly or industrial noise all those gives clues to infer the surrounding circumstances for the listeners. Furthermore, the continuous transformation of the sound image as a medium between landscapes implies the movement of audiences from a certain place to another, as if they are walking through the virtual places composed by Harrison, which reinforces the evolutionary process of the work. 

This methodology was taken into account of in the compositional process of the Ambisonic piece, Teles (2019).

 

[1] Denis Smalley, ‘Defining Transformations’, Interface, 22/2 (1993), pp.279-300 (p.279).

[2] Roads, Composing Electronic Music: A New Aesthetic, p.115.

[3] Ibid., p.111-117.

[4] Ibid., p.149.

[5] Ibid., p.150.

[6] Denis Smalley, ‘Spectro-Morphology and Structuring Processes’, in Simon Emmerson (ed.), The Language of Electroacoustic Music (London: The Macmillan Press Ltd, 1986), pp.61-93 (p.67).

[7] Michael McNabb, ‘Dreamsong: The Composition’, Computer Music Journal, 5/4 (1981), pp.36-53 (p.36).

[8] Leigh Landy, Understanding the Art of Sound Organisation (Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press, 2007), p. 134.

[9] Kim, ‘Imaginal Listening: a quaternary framework for listening to electroacoustic music and phenomena of sound-images’, p.45.

[10] Simon Emmerson, ‘The analysis of live and interactive electroacoustic music: Hans Tutschku – Zellen-Linien (2007), in Simon Emmerson and Leigh Landy (ed.), Expanding the Horizon of Electroacoustic Music Analysis, (Cambridge University Press, 2016), pp.333-354 (p.342).

1.4. Virtual space and sound representation

The field of articulating space has shifted audiences' focus from the traditional stage-centred music performance to immersive audio-visual environments using not only virtual or augmented reality, but also exhibitions using projection mapping and interactive sounds.[1] Immersive sound can give a spatial illusion reinforced by interactions with the physical movement of listeners, which can broaden compositional options in spatiality. Particularly, Barrett insisted that this is the advantage of Higher-Order Ambisonics (HOA) in electroacoustic music composition. First of all, the Ambisonic microphone can capture the sound field in 360 degrees without a complex recording system. The recorded format can be decoded into various forms—not only into huge loudspeakers systems but also stereo or binaural, which is more convenient for monitoring purposes. Also, they can consider the space independently from loudspeakers. Using higher order Ambisonics, larger and relatively stable listening areas are formed. Finally, due to their flexibility, Ambisonics are more practical than Wave Field Synthesis (WFS).[2] A remarkable thing about immersive sound technology is that they play a role of a conductor for all speakers that contribute to not only the spatiality of space (unity), but also the representation of the directions and motions of sound sources (disunity).

Even if it does not result in perfect spatialisation, the more sensory information is delivered to us, the easier it is for us to process. The encouraged immersion in sonic space may give a major influence on our concentration and appreciation of the works. For example, there are studies showing that the combination of vision and auditory sense enables us to classify quickly, even though the visual information is irrelevant.[3] Landy insisted that this synesthetic congruency affects today's art forms.[4] Assistance in meaning making comes from the recognisable sounds and visual information, but we are also aided by the broad scope of our compositional process, even if the stimuli are ambiguous and uncorrelated. In other words, sonic landscape as a place can be more effectively revealed to us by abundant and colourful sensory stimulation in electroacoustic music.

Most of the works in this portfolio are presented by multichannel and immersive sound representation and some of those include visual media.

 

[1] Roads, Composing Electronic Music: A New Aesthetic, p.274.

[2] Natasha Barrett, ‘Composing Images in Space: Schaeffer’s Allure projected in Higher-Order Ambisonics’, International Computer Music Conference (ICMC) (New York, 18th June 2019).

[3] Alberto Gallace and Charles Spence, ‘Multisensory synesthetic interactions in the speeded classification of visual size’, Perceptions & Psychophysics, 68/7 (2006), pp.1191-1203 (p.1200).

[4] Landy, Understanding the Art of Sound Organisation, p.159

2. Compositions

To be updated...

Conclusion

As a composer and listener approaching music composed with a wide range of sound sources, as in the electroacoustic medium, I often consider the ways in which I imagine the perceived sounds spatially. While space as an aspect of electroacoustic composition has been explored in various ways, this commentary summarises the diverse spatial perceptions that I imagined through listening to the music. I believe that this spatial deduction plays a key role in the appreciation and meaning making for both composers and listeners. In this research, rather than simply imagining space as place, I examined the extended concept of space by looking into the primitive process of our spatial reasoning from sound itself. In other words, I proposed that we perceive all sounds spatially, that the space-forming process is the beginning of spatial reasoning, and in the end, that schema which is the association of our senses to all our experiences, comes to us as a place by our imagination.

Language is expressed as a place in my piece, Atmosphere. The function of language as sound transcends the role of simple communication by changing the user's world of consciousness. Likewise, the combinational notion of musical instruments and the instrumental playing technique give a unique sense of spatial description in Nong-hyun and S Quartet. In the above three pieces, the schematic association of language and instrument changed the acoustic places into new spatial perceptions by integration and disintegration of sound. On the other hand, electroacoustic medium and multichannel representation systems expanded the spatial possibilities of the instrument in Echoes. Similarly, the stage was expanded to not only performing space but also listening and interacting space as a sound installation in Connected Islands. Finally, A trace of directionality describes our place living in spatial movement by describing the constant directivity of human beings. Teles invited the listener to various places through the combination of objects exploiting the schema of each place, which described and suggested a sonic journey.

As can be seen from the PhD portfolio above, my compositional strategies show

various formats including fixed media acousmatic, audio-visual, soundscape composition, live interactive performances with visuals, instruments and installation. The various expressions and unique manners have broadened and enriched my perspective so that I can look at space and place as well as the process of my compositional development in a new way.

As Casey said, the technology has created a place where you can link to regardless of the geographical location, that is, the ‘placeless place’. In addition, human beings desire diversity of places due to “the encroachment of an indifferent sameness-of-place on a global scale”.[1] This can be full of suggestions for the electroacoustic art-form. Unlike general music, electroacoustic music and the related performance practices can create various virtual places in between realism and surrealism. [2][3] Moreover, the attempts to create virtual spaces in the music by multichannel and immersive sound representation are leading the interests of today’s electroacoustic culture, like the visual arts have drawn the audiences’ attention.[4] These two features certainly promote the artist’s creativity. I believe that music is an art that can satisfy the active desire to experience a particularity for the audience living in a social uniformity and it will remain and be remembered as a unique ‘placeless place’.

 

[1] Casey, The Fate of Place: A Philosophical History, p.xiv-xv.

[2] Emmerson, ‘The Relation of Language to Materials’, p.23-24.

[3] Denis Smalley, ‘The Listening Imagination: Listening in the Electroacoustic Era’, Contemporary Music Review, 13/2 (1996), pp.77–107 (pp.101-102).

[4] Blesser and Salter, Spaces Speak, Are You Listening? Experiencing Aural Architecture, p.363.

Bibliography

  • Bachelard, Gaston, The Poetics of Space, (New York: Penguin Books, 2014/2nd edn).

  • Blesser, Barry, and Linda-Ruth Salter, Spaces Speak, Are You Listening?: Experiencing Aural Architecture (Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press, 2007).

  • Bridger, Michael, Narrativisation in Electroacoustic and Computer Music - Reflections on Empirical Research into Linsteners' Response (Ann Arbor, MI: Michigan Publishing, University of Michigan Library, 1993).

  • Casey, Edward S., The Fate of Place: A Philosophical History, (Berkeley, Los Angeles, California: University of California Press, 1997).

  • Edwardes, Alistair and Purves, Ross S., 'A Theoretical Grounding for Semantic Descriptions', in J. Mark Ware, George E. Taylor (ed.), Web and Wireless Geographical Information Systems, 7th International Symposium, (Cardiff: Springer, 2007), pp.106–20.

  • Edwardes, Alistair and Purves, Ross S., 'Exploiting Volunteered Geographic Information to describe Place', GIS Research UK 16th Annual Conference (2008).

  • Emmerson, Simon, ‘The Relation of Language to Materials’, in Simon Emmerson (ed.), The Language of Electroacoustic Music (London: The Macmillan Press Ltd, 1986), pp.17–39.

  • Emmerson, Simon, ‘The analysis of live and interactive electroacoustic music: Hans Tutschku - Zellen-Linien (2007), in Simon Emmerson and Leigh Landy (ed.), Expanding the Horizon of Electroacoustic Music Analysis, (Cambridge University Press, 2016), pp.333–54.

  • Fauconnier, Gilles, and Turner, Mark, The Way We Think: Conceptual Blending and the Mind’s Hidden Complexities(Basic Books, 2002).

  • Gallace, Alberto, and Spence, Charles, 'Multisensory synesthetic interactions in the speeded classification of visual size', Perceptions & Psychophysics, 68/7 (2006), pp.1191–203.

  • Gallagher, Shaun, and Zahavi, Dan, The Phenomenological Mind, 2nd edition (Oxon, New York: Routledge, 2012).

  • Ghosh, Vanessa E., and Asaf Gilboa, ‘What Is a Memory Schema? A Historical Perspective on Current Neuroscience Literature’, Neuropsychologia, 53 (2014), pp.104–14.

  • Keane, David, 'At the Threshold of an Aesthetic', in Simon Emmerson (ed.), The language of Electroacoustic Music(London: The Macmillan Press Ltd, 1986), pp.97–118.

  • Keller, Timothy A., and Just Marcel Adam, ‘Structural and Functional Neuroplasticity in Human Learning of Spatial Routes’, NeuroImage, 125 (2016), pp.256–66.

  • Kendall, Gary S., 'The feeling blend: Feeling and emotion in electroacoustic art’, Organised Sound, 19/2 (2014), pp.192–202.

  • Kendall, Gary S., ‘Listening and Meaning: How a Model of Mental Layers Informs Electroacoustic Music Analysis’, in Simon Emmerson and Leigh Landy (ed.), Expanding the Horizon of Electroacoustic Music Analysis, (Cambridge University Press, 2016), pp.31–57.

  • Kim Suk-Jun, 'Acousmatic Reasoning: An Organised Listening with Imagination', Les Journées d’Informatique Musicale (2010), pp.1–11.

  • Kim Suk-Jun, ‘Imaginal Listening: a quaternary framework for listening to electroacoustic music and phenomena of sound-images, Organised Sound, 15/1 (2010), pp.43–53.

  • Landy, Leigh, Understanding the Art of Sound Organization (Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press, 2007).

  • Lombard, Matthew, and Theresa Ditton, ‘At the Heart of It All: The Concept of Presence’, Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 3/2 (1997).

  • McNabb, Michael, ‘Dreamsong: The Composition’, Computer Music Journal, 5/4 (1981), pp.36–53.

  • Merleau-Ponty, Maurice, Phenomenology of Perception, (Routledge, 2002/2nd edn).

  • Roads, Curtis, Composing Electronic Music: A New Aesthetic (Oxford University Press, 2015).

  • Roginska, Agnieszka and Geluso, Paul, Immersive Sound: The Art and Science of Binaural and Multi-channel Audio(Routledge, 2018).

  • Scherer, K. R., ‘What are emotions? And how can they be measured?’, Social Science Information, 44/4 (2005), pp. 693–727.

  • Smalley, Denis, 'Spectro-Morphology and Structuring Processes’, in Simon Emmerson (ed.), The Language of Electroacoustic Music (London: The Macmillan Press Ltd, 1986), pp.61–93.

  • Smalley, Denis, ‘Defining Transformations’, Interface, 22/2 (1993), pp.279–300.

  • Smalley, Denis, ‘Spectromorphology: Explaining Sound-Shapes’, Organised Sound, 2.2 (1997), pp.107–26.

  • Smalley, Denis, ‘The Listening Imagination: Listening in the Electroacoustic Era’, Contemporary Music Review, 13/2 (1996), pp. 77–107.

  • Smalley, Denis, ‘Space-form and the acousmatic image’, Organised Sound, 12/1 (2007), pp.35–58.

  • Truax, Barry, ‘The Aesthetics of Computer Music: A Questionable Concept Reconsidered’, Organised Sound, 5/3 (2000), pp.119–26.

  • Tuan, Yi Fu, Space and Place: The Perspective of Experience, (the University of Minnesota Press, 2001/8th edn).

  • Turner, Mark, The Artful Mind: Cognitive Science and the Riddle of Human Creativity (Oxford University Press, 2006).

  • Uehlein, Friedrich A., ‘Eidos and Eidetic Variation in Husserl’s Phenomenology’, in Manfred Spitzer et al. (ed.), Phenomenology, Language & Schizophrenia (New York: Springer, 1992), pp.88–102.

  • Wishart, Trevor, ‘Sound Symbols and Landscapes’, in Simon Emmerson (ed.), The Language of Electroacoustic Music(London: The Macmillan Press Ltd, 1986), pp.41–60.

  • Wishart, Trevor and Emmerson, Simon, On Sonic Art (Amsterdam: Harwood Academic Publishers, 1996/1st rev.).

  • Young, John, ‘Forming form’, in Simon Emmerson and Leigh Landy (ed.), Expanding the Horizon of Electroacoustic Music Analysis (Cambridge University Press, 2016), pp.58–79.

bottom of page